There is more and more research published that focuses on possible explanations why living in poverty can have a bad influence on education.
Some earlier posts on the topic:
- Very interesting research: Poverty reduces brainpower needed for navigating other areas of life
- The stress of growing up poor can have an impact on the brain functions as an adult (research)
And a possible solution:
Now new research conducted at Northwestern University has taken the finding that linked a mother’s educational background to her children’s literacy and cognitive abilities in a neuroscientific direction: linking poor processing of auditory information in the adolescent brain to a lower maternal educational background.
From the press release:
This study builds on evidence that children from low-income families experience a type of auditory impoverishment. The landmark study by Hart and Risley (1995) revealed that children in high-income families are exposed to 30 million more words than children from families on welfare. This reduction in the quality and quantity of language input, along with greater exposure to unstructured sound such as ambient noise, may be affecting how the brain represents auditory information.
In urban populations, income and amount of noise exposure are known to be correlated. Consistent with the idea that noisy auditory environments increase neural noise, the new Journal of Neuroscience study found that the adolescents from the lower maternal educational group have increased neural activity in the absence of sound input.
According to the study, “Neural models indicate that when the input to a neuron is noisier, the firing rate becomes more variable, ultimately limiting the amount of sensory information that can be transmitted.”
“If your brain is creating a different signal each time you hear a sound, you might be losing some of the details of the sound,” said Skoe, lead author of the study. “Losing these details may create challenges in the classroom and other noisy settings.”
The new research conducted at Northwestern contributes to a recent wave of neuroscientific research demonstrating that sociocultural factors influence brain structure and function.
Another recently published study from the Kraus lab showed that inconsistent neural responses to sounds relate to poor reading but that by acoustically augmenting the classroom, neural responses became more stable.
“Modifying the auditory world for a particular student, even if just for a portion of the day, may improve academic performance and fine-tune how sound is automatically encoded in the brain,” Skoe said.
Ongoing work in Northwestern’s Auditory Neuroscience Laboratory is investigating whether auditory enrichment in the form of music education and other school-based activities can offset the negative impact of an impoverished acoustic environment.
For the new study, brain activity of Chicago Public School adolescents, almost all ninth-graders, was assessed both in response to and in the absence of auditory input. The nervous system’s responses to speech sounds were observed through passive electrophysiological recordings, with students grouped according to the highest educational level achieved by their mothers.
The responses reflect activity from a communication hub within the central nervous system that provides a snapshot of sensory, cognitive and reward circuits that are engaged to process sound. These fundamental, automatic responses to sound reflect past and ongoing sensory experiences and relate to linguistic and cognitive function.
The collection protocol for “the impoverished auditory brain” lasted roughly 20 minutes, during which participants sat comfortably watching a self-selected subtitled movie, while the brain response to speech syllables was passively collected.
The syllables were presented at a rapid rate to the right ear through an earphone placed in the ear canal. The left ear remained unblocked, making the movie sound track audible yet not intense enough to mask the stimulus.
The syllables chosen are common to many languages of the world, and their acoustic characteristics are perceptually challenging.
In addition, IQ assessments for the students were collected, and they were administered a standardized, age-normed test battery of reading ability and executive function (working memory). Previous work has revealed that the neurobiological systems mediating higher order functions such as language, memory and executive function are especially sensitive to disparities in socioeconomic status.
“By studying socioeconomic status within a neuroscientific framework, we have the potential to expand our understanding of the biological signatures of poverty,” Kraus concluded. “And a better understanding of how experiences shape the brain could inform educational efforts aimed at closing the socioeconomic achievement gap.”
The research will be published later today, I’ll update with a link as soon it’s online.
[…] verklaringen? Wel check deze onderzoeken hier, hier, hier en hier […]
[…] The research I mentioned earlier on cognition and poverty is not unknown to this, check here!) […]
[…] Discovering more mechanisms of the influence of poverty on learning: How Poverty Molds the Brain […]
[…] brought here earlier on, stress early in life can have lasting impacts on the brain and that eg poverty molds the brain. So for children, stress can go a long way. A little bit of stress is not necessarily a bad thing […]
[…] Discovering more mechanisms of the influence of poverty on learning: How Poverty Molds the Brain […]
[…] Still the study is in line with some other research, check e.g. here. […]
[…] you think you’ve read this before, you have. Check e.g. this post or this post. But again a study shows the negative effect of poverty-related stress on the […]