This new study by Jani Erola et al European Sociological Review is very interesting. It shows that both genes and unique environment matter more among the advantaged for education and income. The study is based on Finnish register-based data on 6,529 pairs of twins born between 1975 and 1986.
Read these excerpts from the conclusion of the paper:
In summary, our study highlights five findings. First, our baseline findings for education, occupational status, and income show that the relative importance of shared environmental influences was negligible.
…
Second, we find that genetic influences are strongest among the most advantaged families. This partly confirms our first hypothesis: There is no linear relationship between the strength of genetic influences and the quality of the family environment, and the differences between the other groups of families are small. Thus, the enhancement mechanism seems to work principally at the top end of the social spectrum.
…
Third, the social stratification of genetic influences is to some extent depending on the age at which parental SES is observed. In contrast to our expectations, parental social background measured early during childhood led to weaker interactions with genetic influences. This finding is an important addition to previous research on the role of socioeconomic rearing environment at different stages of the early life course. It suggests that the average contribution SES would be more or less constant across childhood and youth (Erola, Jalonen and Lehti, 2016). If gene-environment interactions were not taken into account, we would miss the life-course-specific pattern. It may be that parents have not reached their final level of socioeconomic attainment during children’s early childhood, and once parents have achieved that, their status reflects more accurately their genetic potential. If this is the case, the differences we observe in the association between family background and genetic influences according to children’s age can follow from gene–environment correlation related to parent’s socioeconomic attainment.
…
Fourth, in line with our third hypothesis, we found that the contribution of socioeconomic parental characteristics to genetic influences is stronger the earlier the maturity of an outcome is reached. More specifically, parental characteristics matter mostly for the genetic influences in education, and for occupational standing mostly because it is mediated by their children’s education. Notably, in the case of income, stratification by parental characteristics was weak even before their children’s own education was considered. This is striking: It suggests that nearly all of the factors behind parents’ success or failure in terms of their observed socioeconomic outcomes cannot on average explain that much of how their children succeed economically by age 32–36.
Finally, the results showed the stronger importance of the non-shared environment among the children of parents of high SES. This result was consistent across the three outcomes as well as the indicators of parental SES, and aligned with previous studies showing that socioeconomic outcomes within families differ more strongly among advantaged children (Goldstein and Warren, 2000; Heflin and Pattillo, 2006). A possible explanation can be borrowed from research on stratified parenting (Lareau, 2011; Kalil, Ryan and Corey, 2012) showing that parents of higher social status make more child-specific investments based on their children’s individual talents or particular weaknesses that can accentuate differences among their children (Baier, 2019). However, similar findings could also result from the multiplicative processes if advantaged parents or the children themselves prefer differential treatment. For example, the same innate talent in math could lead to different educational and career pathways and could encourage careers in either business or academia.
Abstract of the study:
To what extent are differences in education, occupational standing, and income attributable to genes, and do genetic influences differ by parents’ socioeconomic standing? When in a children’s life course does parents’ socioeconomic standing matter for genetic influences, and for which of the outcomes, fixed at the different stages of the attainment process, do they matter most? We studied these research questions using Finnish register-based data on 6,529 pairs of twins born between 1975 and 1986. We applied genetically sensitive variance decompositions and took gene–environment interactions into account. Since zygosity was unknown, we compared same-sex and opposite-sex twins to estimate the proportion of genetic variation. Genetic influences were strongest in education and weakest in income, and always strongest among those with the most advantaged socioeconomic background, independent of the socioeconomic indicator used. We found that the shared environment influences were negligible for all outcomes. Parental social background measured early during childhood was associated with weaker interactions with genetic influences. Genetic influences on children’s occupation were largely mediated through their education, whereas for genetic influences on income, mediation through education and occupational standing made little difference. Interestingly, we found that non-shared environment influences were greater among the advantaged families and that this pattern was consistent across outcomes. Stratification scholars should therefore emphasize the importance of the non-shared environment as one of the drivers of the intergenerational transmission of social inequalities.